Keywords
brain-to-brain interaction; entanglement; support vector machines
brain-to-brain interaction; entanglement; support vector machines
The main revisions to Version 2 are:
See the authors' detailed response to the review by James Lake
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Sam Schwarzkopf
Brain-to-brain interaction (BBI) at distance, that is, outside the range of the five senses, has been demonstrated by Pais-Vieira et al., (2013), by connecting the brains of rats via an Internet connection.
A similar effect has been demonstrated with humans in a pilot study by Rao & Stocco, (2013) by sending the EEG activity generated by a subject imagining to move his right hand via the Internet to the brain of a distant partner which triggered his motor cortex causing the right hand to press a key. Similarly, Grau et al., (2014) were able to induce the conscious perception of light flashes to a participant, triggering a robotized transcranial magnetic stimulation by a signal generated from the EEG correlates of the voluntary motor imagery from a partner located 5,000 miles apart and transmitted via the Internet.
Even though there is cultural resistance in accepting the possibility of observing similar effects in humans without an internet connection, some evidence of these effects nevertheless exists. A comprehensive search of all studies related to this line of research has revealed at least eighteen studies from 1974 until the present time (see Supplementary Material).
In all these studies the principal aim was to observe whether the brain activity evoked by a stimulus (e.g. by presenting light flashes or images) in one member of a couple, could also be observed in the brain of the partner. Even if some of these studies, those using functional neuroimaging, can be criticized for potential methodological weaknesses that could account for the reported effects (Acunzo et al., 2013), the questions is still open as to whether or not it is possible to connect two human brains at distance.
The possibility of connecting the brains of two humans at distance without using any classical means of transmission is theoretically expected if it is assumed that two brains, and consequently two minds, can be entangled in a quantum-like manner. In quantum physics, entanglement is a physical phenomenon that occurs when pairs (or groups) of particles interact in ways such that the measurement (observation) of the quantum state (e.g. spin state) of each member is correlated with the others, irrespective of their distance without apparent classical communication.
At present, generalizability from physics variables to biological and mental variables can be done only by analogy given the differences in their properties, but some theoretical models are already available. For example in the Generalized Quantum Theory (Filk & Römer, 2011; Von Lucadou & Romer, 2007; Walach & von Stillfried, 2011), “entanglement can be expected to occur if descriptions of the system that pertain to the whole system are complementary to descriptions of parts of the system. In this case the individual elements within the system, that are described by variables complementary to the variable describing the whole system, are non-locally correlated”.
Reasoning by analogy, we hypothesized the possibility of entangling two minds, and consequently two brains as complementary parts of a single system and studying their interactions at distance without any classical connections.
In a pilot study, Tressoldi et al., (2014) tested five couples of participants with a long friendship and a capacity to maintain a focused mental concentration, who were separated by a distance of approximately five meters without any sensorial contact. Three sequences of silence-signal events lasting two and half minutes and one minute, respectively, were delivered to the first member of the pair. The second member of the pair was simply requested to connect mentally with his/her partner. A total of fifteen pairs of data were analyzed. By using a special classification algorithm, these authors observed an overall percentage of correct coincidences of 78%, ranging from 100% for the first two segments silence-signal, to approximately 43% in the last two. The percentages of coincidences in the first five segments of the protocol were above 80%. Furthermore a robust statistically significant correlation was observed in all but beta EEG frequency bands, but was much stronger in the alpha band.
These preliminary results of the pilot study prompted us to devise this pre-registered replication study.
In line with the recommendations to distinguish exploratory versus confirmatory experiments (Nosek, 2012; Wagenmakers et al., 2012), we pre-registered this study in the Open Science Framework site (https://osf.io/u3yce).
Seven healthy adults, five males and two females, were selected for this experiment and included as co-author. Their mean age was 41.7, SD = 16.6. Inclusion criteria were their friendship lasting more than five years and their experience in maintaining a focused mental concentration resulting from their experience (ranging from four to fifteen years) in meditation and other practices to control mental activity, e.g. martial arts practices, yoga, etc.
Participation inclusion followed the ethical guidelines in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, prot.n.63, 2012, the institution of the main author. Before taking part in the experiment, each participant provided written consent after reading a brief description of the experiment.
Ad-hoc software written in C++ for Windows 7, designed by one of the co-authors, SM, controlled the delivering of the choice of the protocols of stimulation and the timing of the EEG activity recordings of the two partners. EEG activity was recorded by using two Emotiv® EEG Neuroheadsets connected wirelessly to two personal computers running Windows 7 OS and synchronized with the atomic clock. The Emotiv® EEG Neuroheadset technical characteristics are 14 EEG channels based on the International 10–20 locations (AF3, F7, F3, FC5, T7, P7, O1, O2, P8, T8, FC6, F4, F8, AF4, plus 2 references), one mastoid (M1) sensor acts as a ground reference point to which the voltage of all other sensors is compared. The other mastoid (M2) is a feed-forward reference that reduces external electrical interference. Sampling rate is 128 Hz, bandwidth 0.2–45 Hz with digital notch filters at 50 and 60 Hz. Filtering is made by a build in digital 5th order sinc filter and connectivity is obtained by a proprietary wireless connection at the 2.4 GHz band.
One auditory clip was delivered binaurally at a high volume (80 dBs) to one of the partners through Parrot ZIK® headphones connected with the PC controlling the stimulus delivery and EEG recordings. This clip, reproducing a baby crying, was selected among the list of the worst sounds (Cox, 2008) in order to enhance the EEG activity of the stimulated person.
In contrast to the pilot study, the stimulation protocol consisted of three different sequences of 30 seconds of listening to the auditory clip interspersed by silent periods lasting one minute (in the pilot study the durations were twice this length). The three sequences comprised 3, 5 and 7 segments (i.e. silence-signal-silence-signal-silence-signal-silence) and were selected by a random algorithm using the rand function of C++ (in the pilot study only a sequence of 7 segments was used). To prevent any possible prediction of the start of the sequence of events, its presentation was randomly delayed by 1, 2 or 3 minutes.
We devised a procedure aimed at recreating a real situation when there is an important event to share, in this case a communication relating to a baby crying. In order to isolate the two partners, four of them were located in a laboratory of the Department of General Psychology of Padova University and the remaining three were placed in the EvanLab a private laboratory located in Florence, approximately 190 km away. A research assistant was present at each location.
The partner designated as “sender” received the following instructions: “when ready, you must concentrate in silence for one to three minutes to relax and prepare to receive the stimulation to send to your partner. To facilitate your mental connection with him/her, you will see a photo of his/her face via the special glasses (virtual glasses model Kingshop OV2, see Figure S1 in the Supplementary Material). Your only task is to endeavor to send him/her mentally what you will hear, reducing your body and head movements in order to reduce artifacts. You will hear a sequence of a baby crying lasting 30 seconds, separated by one minute intervals. The experiment will last approximately 10 minutes”.
The instructions to the second partner designated as “receiver” were: “when ready, you must concentrate in silence for one to three minutes to relax and prepare to receive the stimulation sent by your partner. To facilitate your mental connection with him/her, you will see a photo of his/her face via the special glasses. Your task is to connect with him/her mentally attempting to receive the stimulation he/she is hearing, reducing your body and head movements in order to reduce artifacts. The experiment will last approximately 10 minutes”.
When all devices were set up, the “sender” was continuously presented with the image of the “receiver” except when the signal was delivered. In this case, the image of a baby crying associated with the auditory clip, replaced the previous one. On the contrary, the “receiver” was continuously presented with only the image of the “sender” up to the end of the session without any further auditory and visual cues that could inform him/her about what the “sender” perceived and listened.
After both partners gave their approval to begin the experiment, the main research assistant located in the EvanLab, started the experiment by informing the second research assistant connected via the Internet to trigger the software controlling the experiment. At the end of the experiment, both partners were informed that it was over. After a break, the partners reversed their roles if available.
Pairing each participant located in one laboratory with each participant located in the second laboratory, a total of 22 pairs of data were collected, because two participants contributed to only three sessions. Two pairs of data were eliminated due to a faulty recording of the EEG activity.
The BrainScanner™ classification software was originally developed and is available from one of the co-authors P.F. (Pasquale Fedele p.fedele@liquidweb.it). The analysis was carried out offline separately for each pair taking as input the raw data recorded by the two Emotiv® EEG Neuroheadsets using the procedure and parameters which yielded the best classification accuracy in the pilot study. The first analysis was a classical principal component analysis (PCA) to reduce the data obtained by the fourteen channels to their latent variables. Fifty percent of these data, randomly sampled related to all signal and silence segments were used for the training of the C-supported vector classification (C-SVC) machine (Chang & Lin, 2011; Steinwart & Christmann, 2008).
Regarding the kernel choice, the one that gave the best performance during the pilot tests was the RBF (radial basis function). A general description of the Supported vector machines (SVMs) is reported in the Supplementary Material.
After the training phase, the algorithm was ready to generalize the obtained classification model to all the data, matching the sequence of events of the stimulation protocol with the whole EEG activity. The result was a contingency table (see examples in Figure 1). To control the reliability of these results, the whole procedure was repeated five times and the results were identical.
The first row of each example shows the timing and the sequence of periods of silence and stimulation as delivered to the “sender” brain. The first row of each example shows the timing and the sequence of periods of silence and stimulation as delivered to the “sender” brain. The second row of each example shows the timing and the sequence of the periods of silence and stimulation identified by the BrainScanner™ classifier in the “receiver” brain. Red color = silence; Black color = signal. The first example represents what it is expected if there was no mental connection, the second and the third one represents what we would have observed with mental connection. Using the criteria to consider a coincidence a segment of the protocol with at least one timing boundary (initial or final) overlapped between the two rows, in the second example we count 6 coincidences and 1 omission and in the third example 5 coincidences and 2 omissions.
From the contingency table of each participant with the role of “receiver” we counted the number of coincidences and the number of errors and missing. Given our interest in detecting the sequence of binary events (silence-signal) and not their absolute overlap, a signal detected in the EEG activity of the receiver was considered as a coincidence if at least one of its boundaries (initial or final) overlapped with that of the sender (see examples in Figure 1).
To check the reliability of the scoring system, the data were analyzed independently by two co-authors, PT and SM. Their overall agreement was 89.3%; discrepancies were solved re-checking the original data. All the individual raw data and results are available for independent analyses at http://figshare.com/articles/BBI_Confirmatory/1030617.
To have convergent evidence of the relationship between the EEG activity of the two partners, we correlated their EEG activity related to the signal and silence periods recorded in the fourteen channels, with respect to the five frequency bands, delta, theta, alpha, beta and gamma normalized with respect to the total power. Each period of silence and stimulation was divided into tracts of 4 seconds and the Power Spectral Density (PSD) was computed by the periodogram method. The five spectral bands were distinguished as follows: delta (0.5–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–15 Hz), beta (15–30 Hz) and gamma (30–45 Hz). The PSD of the different bands collapsing all silence and signal periods, was then expressed in normalized units by dividing the power in each band by the sum of the powers in all the bands.
To test the significance of the correlation coefficient we adopted a distribution-free approach, the bivariate non-parametric bootstrap (Bishara & Hittner, 2012) with 5000 iterations. From the sampling distribution, we computed the 95% confidence interval following the percentile method. The bivariate test rejects the null hypothesis if r = 0 is not included within the confidence interval. The overall results are reported in Table 2 whereas the results of each of the 20 pairs are reported in Supplementary Table S1. The raw data and the software source code in MatLab “Accardo_Confirmatory_rev.m” are available at http://figshare.com/articles/BBI_Confirmatory/1030617.
Instead of a traditional Null Hypothesis Significant Testing, we adopted a frequentist parameter estimation approach in line with the APA (2008) and the statistical reform recommendation (Cumming, 2014) and a Bayesian models comparison approach as suggested by Wagenmakers et al., (2011).
The numbers of coincidences in the EEG activity of the participants with the role of “receiver” (the data of the participants with the role of “sender” are irrelevant in this case) detected by the BrainScanner™ classifier, related to the three different stimulation protocols in the twenty sessions are reported in Table 1a, Table 1b and Table 1c. The expected number of coincidences is zero. A percentage of coincidences of the silence and signal events well above the number of missing values and errors, can be a demonstration of a brain (mind) connections between the pairs of participants unless statistical or procedural artifacts can explain them.
No. 9 | Silence | Signal | Silence | % Detection Accuracy |
---|---|---|---|---|
Silence | 9 | 100 | ||
Signal | 9 | 100 | ||
Silence | 9 | 100 |
No. 8 | Silence | Signal | Silence | Signal | Silence | % Detection Accuracy |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Silence | 8 | 100 | ||||
Signal | 8 | 100 | ||||
Silence | 7 | 87.5 | ||||
Signal | 3 | 37.5 | ||||
Silence | 2 | 25 |
No. 3 | Silence | Signal | Silence | Signal | Silence | Signal | Silence | % Detection Accuracy |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Silence | 3 | 100 | ||||||
Signal | 3 | 100 | ||||||
Silence | 3 | 100 | ||||||
Signal | 2 | 66.7 | ||||||
Silence | 2 | 66.7 | ||||||
Signal | 1 | 33.3 | ||||||
Silence | 0 | 0 |
The overall percentages of coincidences and their precision were estimated with the corresponding confidence intervals. The classification algorithm correctly detected 69/88 = 78.4%; 95% CI: 68.7–85.7 events, 26/34 = 76.4%; 95% CI: 58.4–87.5 related to the signals and 43/54 = 79.6%; 95% CI: 67.1–88.2 related to the silence events.
Furthermore the Bayes Factor comparing the hypothesis that the percentage of coincidences will outperform the percentage of errors and missing data with the hypothesis of null difference between these two percentages, was calculated with the online applet available at http://pcl.missouri.edu/bf-binomial, using a uniform prior probability distribution based on a beta distribution.
The corresponding Bayes Factors comparing the H1 (above chance detection) vs H0 (chance detection) hypothesis, for the overall and the signal coincidences are 390,625 and 27.1 respectively.
It is interesting to observe that for all three stimulation protocols, the percentages of coincidences of the first three events (silence-signal-silence) was 98.3%, dropping to 40.9% for the next two events (signal-silence) and to 16.6% for the last two events (signal-silence). This drop was also observed in the pilot study, even if it was less dramatic: 83.3% and 43.3%, respectively. However it is important to recall that in the pilot study, the duration of the signals and the silence periods were 60 seconds and 180 seconds, respectively. A plausible explanation of this difference can be the limitation of the present version of our classifier to extract sufficient information to differentiate the two classes of events from the EEG activity, postulating that the signal/noise ratio of EEG activity reduced after a sequence of three events.
To control if the above results might represent true coincidences between the EEG activity of the pairs of participants or simply the results of the BrainScanner™ efficiency in detecting sequences of differential EEG activity in the participants with the role of “receivers”, we redid all comparisons by training the BrainScanner™ with the EEG activity of the participants with the role of “senders” generalizing the obtained classification model to the EEG activity of the participants with the role of “receivers”. The outputs of this new comparison are available in the file NewCoincidences at http://figshare.com/articles/BBI_Confirmatory/1030617.
Only five pairs, 8,11,13,14 and 15, revealed coincidences in at least the initial part of the sequence of the events.
As a further control on whether these coincidences could be the result of the specific pair of participants or if they might have been obtained even with a different partner, we compared the data of each of the five “senders” with the data of all their nineteen unpaired “receivers”.
For pair 13 we obtained similar or better results with “receivers” 7,8,10,14,16,17,18. For pair 8 we obtained identical results only with the “receiver” 10. For pairs 11,14 and 15 we did not obtained better or more precise coincidences. See results in Figure 2.
First row: the sequence of events of the protocol of stimulation; second row: sequence of events detected in the EEG activity of the “sender”; third row: sequence of events detected in the EEG activity of the “receiver” by using the classification model obtained analyzing the EEG activity of the “sender”.
The Pearson’s r correlation values with corresponding 95% CIs between the silence and signal events of each of the twenty pairs of participants separately for the five frequency bands, are reported in the Table S1 (see Supplementary Material). The corresponding graphs are available at http://figshare.com/articles/BBI_Confirmatory/1030617.
In Figure 3, we report the alpha band normalized power spectrum values recorded in the fourteen channels of the EEG activity of pair 15 as an example of strong correlation.
Legend: S = Sender; R = Receiver.
The overall average correlations among the twenty pairs transformed by using the Fisher r-to-z transformation formula, was estimated with 5000 bootstrap resamplings with the corresponding confidence intervals for each EEG frequency band, separately for the silence and signal events. The results are reported in Table 2.
Statistically significant correlations are colored in bold.
As observed in the pilot study, we found reliable correlations in the alpha band for both silence and signal events and in the gamma band only for the silence events. In the pilot study we also observed the strongest correlation in the alpha band.
Fourteen out of the twenty pairs of participants showed statistically significant correlations in at least one of these two frequency bands.
To control whether these correlations were specific to the pair of participants or might have also been obtained with their unpaired partners, we redid the correlation related to the Alpha and Gamma bands pairing each “sender” with all nineteen unpaired “receivers”. The results were the follow: Alpha silence: 0.35; 95% CI: 0.25,0.43; Alpha signal: 0.27; 95% CI: 0.17,0.36; Gamma silence: 0.35; 95% CI: 0.25,0.43; Gamma signal: 0.14; 95% CI: 0.04,0.23.
Compared with the pilot study of Tressoldi et al., (2014), in the present study the pairs of participants were approximately 190 km away each other, the length of the sequence of events was randomized and the durations of the silence and signal periods were halved. However, the percentage of the overall correct sequences of events were almost identical with those observed in the pilot study. In the pilot study, the overall percentage of correct identification of the events was 78%; 95% CI=72–87 with respect to the 78.4%; 95% CI=68.7–85.7, observed in the present study.
Furthermore the average correlation estimated with 5000 bootstrap resamplings among all pairs of data was 0.58; 95% CI=0.46–0.69 and 0.55; 95% CI=0.43–0.65 for the alpha band respectively for the silence and the signal periods in the pilot study and 0.32; 95% CI=0.18–0.44, for silence and 0.27; 95% CI=0.13–0.40, for signal events in this confirmatory study. For the gamma band, the correlation values were 0.36; 95% CI=0.24–0.49 and 0.32; 95% CI=0.19–0.46 for the silence and signal, respectively, in the pilot study and 0.23; 95% CI=0.10–0.37 and 0.12; 95% CI=-0.009–0.26 in the present study.
The differences in the strength of correlations between the pilot and the present study may well be explained by the reduction of fifty percent in the duration of the silence and signal events with a consequent increment of the signal/noise ratio.
The alpha band is a marker of attention (Klimesh, 2012; Klimesch et al., 1998), whereas the gamma band is a marker of mental control as typically observed during meditation (Cahn et al., 2010; Lutz et al., 2004) and in this case the correlations we have observed could represent an EEG correlate of the synchronized attention between the pairs of participants.
We think that these results are mainly due to the innovative classification algorithm devised for this line of investigation and the enrolment of participants selected for their long friendship and experience in maintaining a mental concentration on the task. The drop of coincidences after three segments, corresponding to approximately five minutes, could be a limit of our classification algorithm to detect the differences between silence and signal, because of an increase of exogenous and endogenous EEG noise correlated to fatigue and loss of concentration (mental connection) between the two partners.
Effects not related to mental interaction at distance or artifacts that could explain our results
The large distance between the pair of participants excludes any sensorial connections between them. The only possibilities of artificial connections between the EEG activity of the pairs of participants could be caused by sensorial triggers sent to the participant with the role of “receiver” by the computer recording his/her EEG activity. This possibility was excluded because the randomization, both of the start of the delivery of the protocol and of the length of sequences of events, was controlled only by the computer connected with the EEG activity of the participant with the role of “transmitter” and no acoustic or visual events were associated with these computations. Another possible source of artifacts could derive from the research assistants managing the computers connected with the EEG activity of the two participants. In this case the only possibility of synchronizing the EEG of the two participants could be obtained if the research assistant who randomized the type of the sequence of events sent this information to the research assistant of the “receiver” who sent auditory signals to influence the EEG activity of the “receiver”. All our research assistants were part of the research team and this possibility can be excluded with certainty.
Another potential cause of the observed correlations between the stimulation protocol and the EEG activity of the participants with the role of “receivers”, could be due to their capacity to self-induce a synchronization of their EEG activity with the timing of the protocol delivered to the “sender” partner, predicting when it started, after 1, 2 or 3 minutes and when a silence or signal period was delivered. Apart from the fact that even if our participants were able to self-induce a differential EEG activity, they could guess the correct timing of the stimulation protocol only for one third of the sessions, and there is no evidence that humans can obtain such as skills for time sequences lasting 60 or 30 seconds. Furthermore, the participants that were also co-authors of this study, specified that they did not engage in such activity.
Could our results be simply artifacts on how we analyzed the data?
The specificity control of the observed correlations between the Alpha and Gamma bands of the pairs of participants, casts doubt on their specificity. In other words, they could also be observed correlating the data of unpaired participants.
The change of the method to detect the coincidences, that is, by using the classification model obtained with the EEG data of the “senders” to measure the coincidences in the sequence of silence and signal events in the EEG of the “receivers” plus the specificity control, reveals potential true coincidences in only four out of the twenty pairs of participants.
Are these results sufficient to support the hypothesis that human minds and their brains, can be connected at distance? Only multiple independent replications and further controls on further potential methodological and statistical artifacts can support this hypothesis both using our data and different participants.
While awaiting new and independent controls and replications of our findings, we are planning to improve the current stimulation protocol to support a simple mental telecommunication code at distance. For example, it is sufficient to associate any small sequence of events with a message, i.e. silence-signal = “CALL ME”; silence-signal-silence = “DANGER”, etc.
The next steps of this line of research are an optimization of the classification algorithm to detect longer sequences of events and the analysis of data online.
figshare: BBI_Confirmatory, doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1030617 (Tressoldi, 2014a).
The BrainScannerTM classification software used in this study is available on request from Pasquale Fedele, email: p.fedele@liquidweb.it.
The ad-hoc software written in C++ for Windows 7 used to control the delivery of the choice of protocols and the timing of the EEG activity recordings is available under a CCBY license from figshare: Mind Sync Data Acquisition Software, doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1108110 (Tressoldi, 2014b).
PT, LP, AF, PC, SM devised the experiment; MB, PF and AA contributed to the software development; PT, LP, AF, PC, SM, DR and FR contributed to the data collection, PT and LP wrote the paper.
We acknowledge the comments and suggestions of the reviewers Sam Schwarzkopf and James Lake to the previous versions. The English was revised by the Proof Reading Service.
Supported vector machines (SVMs) are an example of generalized linear classifiers also defined as maximum margin classifiers because they minimize the empirical error of classification maximizing the margins of separation of the categories. SVMs can be considered as alternative techniques for the learning of polynomial classifiers very different to the classical techniques of neural networks training.
Neural networks with a single layer have an efficient learning algorithm, but they are useful only in the case of linearly separable data. Conversely, multilayer neural networks can represent non-linear functions, but they are difficult to train because of the number of dimensions of the space of weights, and because the most common techniques, such as back-propagation, allow to obtain the network weights by solving an optimization problem not convex and not bound, consequently it presents an indeterminate number of local minima (Basheer & Hajmeer, 2000). The SVM training technique solves both problems: it is an efficient algorithm and is able to represent complex non-linear functions. The characteristic parameters of the network are obtained by solving a convex quadratic programming problem with equality constraints or box type (in which the value of the parameter must be maintained within a range), which provides a single global minimum.
The results of a comprehensive search of all studies related to this line of research revealed at least eighteen studies from 1974 until the present time. These references are presented in a Word document as part of the Supplementary Material.
Values in bold are statistically significant (when the confidence intervals do not include the zero).
Views | Downloads | |
---|---|---|
F1000Research | - | - |
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
|
- | - |
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Invited Reviewers | ||
---|---|---|
1 | 2 | |
Version 3 (revision) 23 Oct 14 |
read | |
Version 2 (revision) 29 Sep 14 |
read | |
Version 1 05 Aug 14 |
read | read |
Provide sufficient details of any financial or non-financial competing interests to enable users to assess whether your comments might lead a reasonable person to question your impartiality. Consider the following examples, but note that this is not an exhaustive list:
Sign up for content alerts and receive a weekly or monthly email with all newly published articles
Already registered? Sign in
The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.
You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.
You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.
If your email address is registered with us, we will email you instructions to reset your password.
If you think you should have received this email but it has not arrived, please check your spam filters and/or contact for further assistance.
In the v2 of the paper and in the replies to ... Continue reading I confirm that the coincidences are determined by comparing the sending protocol and the result of decoding the receiver's EEG.
In the v2 of the paper and in the replies to the 2 reviewers we have clarified this point.
In the v2 of the paper and in the replies to the 2 reviewers we have clarified this point.